Unleash Your Inner Adventurer with Sebco MT Magic Pass

By admin

Sebco MT Magic Pass is a government initiative that aims to provide convenient and efficient transportation services to the citizens of Kerala, India. The Magic Pass is a multi-purpose card that enables users to access various modes of public transportation in the state including buses, ferries, and metro trains. The Magic Pass is a contactless smart card that can be easily recharged with the desired amount of money. It offers several advantages to the users such as easy and quick access to public transportation, cashless transactions, and discounted fares. The card can be used on any bus or ferry operated by the Kerala State Road Transport Corporation (KSRTC) and the Kerala Shipping and Inland Navigation Corporation (KSINC), respectively. One of the main benefits of the Magic Pass is its flexibility.



Sleight-of-hand magic trick only fools monkeys with opposable thumbs

Illusion involving a hidden thumb confounds capuchin and squirrel monkeys for the same reason as humans – it misdirects the expected outcomes of actions they can carry out.

This mirroring in our neural motor system might explain why the French drop worked for the capuchins and squirrel monkeys but not for marmosets

Nicola Clayton

By performing a famous magic trick for three species of monkey with differing hand structures, scientists have discovered that – in order to deceive – a conjuror needs the same anatomy as their audience.

Psychologists used a sleight-of-hand trick called the French drop, in which an object appears to vanish when a spectator assumes it is taken from one hand by the hidden thumb of the other hand.

The study, carried out by the University of Cambridge’s Comparative Cognition Lab, found that monkeys lacking opposable thumbs did not fall for the assumption – staying wise to the whereabouts of tasty treats a magician tried to make disappear.

The research suggest that sharing a biomechanical ability may be necessary for accurately anticipating the movements of those same limbs in other individuals.

This is true even when those apparently accurate predictions end in befuddlement at the hands of an illusionist. The study is published today in the journal Current Biology.

“Magicians use intricate techniques to mislead the observer into experiencing the impossible. It is a great way to study blind spots in attention and perception,” said Dr Elias Garcia-Pelegrin, who has practiced magic for a decade, and conducted the experimental work during his PhD at Cambridge.

“By investigating how species of primates experience magic, we can understand more about the evolutionary roots of cognitive shortcomings that leave us exposed to the cunning of magicians.”

“In this case, whether having the manual capability to produce an action, such as holding an item between finger and thumb, is necessary for predicting the effects of that action in others,” said Garcia-Pelegrin, recently appointed an assistant professor at the National University of Singapore.

The French drop is often the first trick any budding magician sets out to master.

A coin is displayed in one hand. The other hand reaches over and grabs it. The palm of the second hand faces inwards, with the magician’s thumb concealed behind fingers.

The audience knows the thumb is lurking – ready to grip – so assumes the coin has been taken when it is no longer visible. Their attention follows the second hand, only to find it empty at 'the reveal'. The magician had secretly dropped the coin into the palm of the original hand.

Food morsels replaced coins for the monkeys, and were given as rewards – but only if the animals guessed the correct hand. Scientists predicted that monkeys with opposable thumbs would act like human audiences: assume the hidden thumb had grabbed the item, and choose the wrong hand.

They repeatedly performed the French drop on 24 monkeys. Eight capuchins were dazzled with peanuts, eight squirrel monkeys with dried mealworms, and eight marmosets with marshmallows.

Capuchins are famed for dexterity, and use stone tools to crack nuts in the wild. They can waggle each finger, and have opposable thumbs allowing 'precision grip' between thumb and forefingers.

The capuchins were regularly fooled by the French drop (81% of the time). They mostly chose the empty second hand, and experienced a paucity of peanuts as a result.

Squirrel monkeys are much less dextrous than capuchins, with limited thumb rotation, but can oppose their thumbs. As such, they are still familiar with a hidden thumb interacting with fingers. However, they cannot perform a ‘precision grip’ in the same way as capuchins and humans.

Yet squirrel monkeys were routinely misled by vanishing mealworms (93% of the time). “Squirrel monkeys cannot do full precision grips, but they were still fooled. This suggests that a monkey doesn’t have to be expert in a movement in order to predict it, just roughly able to do it,” said Garcia-Pelegrin.

Marmosets do not have opposable thumbs. Their thumbs align with their fingers to make five equidistant digits, ideal for climbing thick tree trunks. Marmosets were rarely taken in by magic (just 6% of the time). They simply chose the hand in which the marshmallow was initially placed, and stuck with it.

Previous work from the Cambridge team shows that species without hands at all, in this case birds from the corvid family, namely Eurasian jays, make similar choices as marmosets when confronted with the French drop.

The team also tried nullifying the tricks by actually completing the hand-to-hand transfers, instead of misdirecting with a French drop. This time, the capuchins and squirrel monkeys anticipated correctly and dined out, and the marmosets missed out.

Finally, the scientists devised their own version of the French drop, which they call the “Power drop”. It utilises a hand action that all the monkey species can perform – essentially a full fist grab. The power drop fooled all of the monkey species the vast majority of the time.

“There is increasing evidence that the same parts of the nervous system used when we perform an action are also activated when we watch that action performed by others,” said Prof Nicola Clayton FRS, senior author of the study from Cambridge’s Department of Psychology.

“This mirroring in our neural motor system might explain why the French drop worked for the capuchins and squirrel monkeys but not for marmosets.”

“It’s about the embodiment of knowledge,” added Clayton. “How one’s fingers and thumbs move helps to shape the way we think, and the assumptions we make about the world – as well as what others might see, remember and anticipate, based on their expectations.”

“Our work raises the intriguing possibility that an individual’s inherent physical capability heavily influences their perception, their memory of what they think they saw, and their ability to predict manual movements of those around them.”

Another co-author of the study, Clive Wilkins, Artist in Residence at Cambridge’s Department of Psychology, is a professional magician and Member of the Magic Circle.


The text in this work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. Images, including our videos, are Copyright ©University of Cambridge and licensors/contributors as identified. All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways – as here, on our main website under its Terms and conditions, and on a range of channels including social media that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.

Orangutans at Miami Zoo Use iPads to Communicate

At the Miami Zoo's Jungle Island, handlers are interacting with orangutans using the iPad. The apes use the tablet to identify items they're familiar with and express their wants and needs.

In this Feb. 21, 2012 photo, an orangutan works with an iPad at Jungle Island in Miami. (AP Photo/J Pat Carter) J Pat Carter

Save this story Save this story

The iPad is by far the most adored tablet in the human world. The numbers speak for themselves. But what you may not know is that members of the animal kingdom dig the 9.7-inch tablet too -- particularly a clan of six orangutans at the Miami Zoo.

At the Miami Zoo's Jungle Island, handlers are interacting with orangutans using the iPad. The apes use the tablet to identify items they're familiar with, and express their wants and needs. This is done primarily through an app designed for autistic children that displays an array of object images onscreen.

"We’ll ask them to identify 'Where’s the coconut?', and they’ll point it out," Linda Jacobs, who oversees the Jungle Island program, told Wired. "We want to build from that and give them a choice in what they have for dinner -- show them pictures of every vegetable we have available that day, and let them pick, giving them the opportunity to have choices."

Orangutans are very intelligent, but lack voice boxes and vocal cords, which can make communication difficult. Up until now, zoo keepers have been using sign language to communicate with them. Using the iPad gives the orangutans another form of communication with humans, provides them with mental stimulation, and also gives those who don't know sign language a chance to interact with humans.

The six orangutans at the Miami Zoo -- 35-year-old Connie, 33-year-old Sinbad, 14-year-old Hannah, 12-year-old Jake, and 8-year-old twin girls Peanut and Pumpkin -- were first introduced to the iPad last summer. Initially, the orangutans were just shown the iPad to desensitize them. Next the trainers asked them to touch the iPad without pulling it into the cage. "They catch on so quickly, it wasn't long before we started showing them pictures and identifying different objects with them," Jacobs said.

Sinbad and Connie aren't so keen on the tablet. "I like to compare the two older ones to my parents -- I keep trying to get them to use an iPad and they’re just not interested," Jacobs said. The other orangutans, though, are very excited by the tablet. They take turns getting to use it, and all run to be the first one to handle it, Jacobs said.

"They really are so intelligent that I think there’s no limit to what they can learn," Jacobs said. "It's just about developing the technology to make it possible."

Due to their curious nature and tough nails, the orangutans don't actually get to hold the iPad (which is housed in an Otter Box case) in the cage themselves. Instead, a trainer holds it outside the cage. If left to their own devices, the primates would take it apart to see how it works, Jacobs said, so it wouldn't last that long.

In the future, Jacobs hopes to set up a form of video conferencing so the apes can meet and interact with their counterparts at other zoos across the world (an organization called Orangutan Outreach is currently working on this with donated iPads at other U.S. zoos). She also hopes that this endeavor will bring more awareness to orangutans, which are greatly endangered in the wild because of deforestation.

Did the Baboon Feel the Magic?

Not yet as popular as cats and dogs in superhero outfits, there is a growing category of YouTube video featuring magicians performing for various animal species. The latest one making the rounds on Facebook is a short clip of a baboon in a zoo enclosure watching a sleight-of hand card vanish through a glass barrier.

The video, only about 10 seconds long, shows a sleepy baboon sitting against the glass divider, clearly bored with the constant stream of visitors. But not for long. From the other side of the glass, a middle-aged man extends his hand. He’s holding a small white piece of paper or plastic, the size of a playing card. The man’s white baseball cap and red polo shirt don’t signal “magician” at first. But this guy is clearly adept at legerdemain. He shows the card to the primate, as if saying “This is a regular card from a regular deck.” At first, no reaction. The baboon’s half-closed eyes stay that way. It really couldn’t care less. After millions of juveniles sticking their tongues out at it, knocking on the glass day in and day out, who cares about yet another tourist, never mind the card brandishing?

Then, a flick of the wrist and POOF! The card is gone. The man shows his empty palm to the monkey. Now’s he got the animal’s attention! The baboon raises his eyebrows in apparent amazement, and a fraction of a second later, lunges at the man, jaws open wide. It is only thanks to the glass barrier that the man gets to keep all his fingers. The monkey looks in the man’s eyes menacingly. “You better produce that card now, or there’ll be hell to pay,” it seems to imply. The magician makes the card re-appear with another wrist snap. The baboon now looks much happier and reaches lightning-like for the card with his hand—but of course, there’s the glass barrier in between. Then the video ends.

There has been extensive online commentary concerning the meaning of the primate’s ostensibly all-too-human reaction. The general consensus appears to be that the baboon felt the magic in much the same way as a human spectator would. There have been some notable exceptions, however: in an interview for Caitlin Roper from WIRED, primatologist Frans de Waals noted that the baboon’s reaction might have been due less to the magic trick and more to unwelcome eye-contact from the human (many primates perceive direct eye-contact as aggression). One problem with that possibility is that the eye contact seems to happen only after the trick and the animal’s pounce (although it’s hard to know for sure when the initial eye-contact occurs, because we never see the man’s face).

Caitlin Roper also contacted me for my impressions on the baboon’s reaction. By the time we got hold of each other, her article had been already published… so you won’t find my opinion there. But this is my hypothesis: the monkey’s reaction was in response to the magic trick, but it was not the same reaction as a human would have had: at least a human over the age of 5.

I have put forward previously that animals’ perception of the conjuring arts –perhaps with the exception of great apes—is different from that from human adults. Sure, animals can be surprised by vanishing objects, just as humans can. But shock is not the only element of magic, albeit it is certainly an important one. There is also awe and enjoyment. Perhaps even happiness: a common reaction to a magic trick is joyful laughter. It’s not that the magic is funny, but that it is delightful.

But why should magic delight us? One possibility is that, just like a benign joke, a magic trick is a type of harmless transgression. You laugh in a magic show because, even though you’ve witnessed the breakage of physical reality, you know you’re safe. If you thought the magic was for real, you might be terrified. Or at least really annoyed.

That baboon did not have a joyous reaction to the magic.

Which brings me back to the issue of magic for adults versus children. A common misconception is that magic is intended for children, when in fact the best magic spectators are adult ones. Children under the age of 5, in particular, do not enjoy magic very much. I have heard magicians express this sentiment many times. The explanations vary: perhaps children’s developing attentional systems are less susceptible to misdirection than adult ones, or maybe young children have not yet developed strong theories about the laws of nature, so everything is equally magical (or non-magical) to them. Whatever the reason, I have had the opportunity to witness this phenomenon first hand. Six or seven years ago, Stephen Macknik and I visited theatrical pickpocket Apollo Robbins in Las Vegas, to plan a research study in which we were collaborating. We brought along our oldest child, then a toddler, for the trip. Apollo was kind enough to demonstrate extraordinary sleight-of-hand magic for our family: he gave our child coins, and then stole them without him (or us) noticing, only to make them re-appear in a different part of the boy’s body. Over and over and over. Steve and I were laughing helplessly, completely enchanted, thoroughly enjoying ourselves.

Our kid? Not so much.

He. Was. Mad. The bad man had given him some coins, and then taken them away for no good reason. What if the man had amazing prestidigitation skills? He had no right to steal those coins! The injustice! Our son started crying in anger. And yes, you guessed it: he kind of looked like a baby baboon.

The views expressed are those of the author(s) and are not necessarily those of Scientific American.

One of the main benefits of the Magic Pass is its flexibility. Users can choose between different types of passes depending on their travel needs. There are daily passes, weekly passes, monthly passes, and even annual passes available.

Sebco mt magic pass

This allows users to save money and enjoy unlimited travel within the validity period of their pass. The Magic Pass also contributes to the sustainability goals of the state government. By encouraging the use of public transportation, it helps reduce congestion on the roads and minimize carbon emissions. It also promotes a greener and cleaner environment for the citizens of Kerala. To ensure ease of use and accessibility, the Sebco MT Magic Pass can be conveniently topped up at various authorized locations across the state including KSRTC and KSINC counters, as well as select shops and outlets. Users can also recharge their cards online via the Sebco official website or through mobile applications. In conclusion, the Sebco MT Magic Pass is a commendable initiative by the government of Kerala to promote the use of public transportation and provide convenient travel options to its citizens. With its range of benefits and flexibility, the Magic Pass is gradually becoming an essential tool for daily commuting in the state..

Reviews for "Unlock Exclusive Access with Sebco MT Magic Pass"

1. Jimmy - 1 star
I was really disappointed with the Sebco mt magic pass. The so-called "magic" effects were incredibly underwhelming and cheaply made. The tricks were easily deciphered, and it took away all the excitement. Additionally, the instructions were not clear, and it was difficult to understand how to perform the tricks properly. Overall, I felt like I wasted my money on this product.
2. Sarah - 2 stars
I purchased the Sebco mt magic pass for my son's birthday, hoping it would provide him with hours of fun and entertainment. Unfortunately, it fell short of expectations. The tricks included in the set were not very impressive, and my son quickly lost interest. The quality of the props was also subpar, with some of them breaking after just a few uses. I would not recommend this product to others looking for a magic set that will truly amaze and engage children.
3. Mark - 1 star
The Sebco mt magic pass was a huge disappointment. The tricks included in the set were not only boring but also poorly explained. The instructions were confusing and lacked detail, making it difficult to understand how to perform the tricks correctly. The quality of the props was also questionable, with some of them falling apart after only a couple of uses. Save your money and look for a better magic set elsewhere.
4. Emily - 2 stars
I was not impressed with the Sebco mt magic pass. The tricks were not as advertised, and they did not capture my interest. It felt like the set was designed for younger children, and it lacked the sophistication and complexity that I was expecting. The props included were flimsy and cheaply made, making it hard to perform the tricks smoothly. Overall, I would not recommend this magic pass to anyone looking for a more engaging and entertaining experience.

Step into a World of Wonder with Sebco MT Magic Pass

Elevate Your Entertainment Experience with Sebco MT Magic Pass